Thursday, December 10, 2020

How your brain decides to put in effort

A man wears a gray knitted cap that looks like a brain

Researchers have clear visual evidence that a region of the human brain known as the ventral striatum kicks in during decision-making to weigh the costs versus the benefits of making a physical effort.

The research gives the first detailed view of ventral striatum activity during three phases of effort-based decision-making—the anticipation of initiating an effort, the actual execution of the effort, and the reward, or outcome, of the effort.

“It’s important to understand the neural mechanisms underlying motivation,” says first author Shosuke Suzuki, a graduate student of psychology at Emory University.

“Our work has wide implications for treatment of disorders related to reduced motivation, such as depression, schizophrenia, and PTSD. It may also help enhance motivational programs for everything from education to athletics and public health.”

Decision mystery

“The willingness to expend effort is something crucial to our survival and something that we use every day,” adds senior author Michael Treadway, a research professor in the psychology department and psychiatry and behavioral science department.

“We’ve identified two closely overlapping, but nevertheless distinct, areas of the ventral striatum involved with different phases of effort-based decision-making,” Treadway says. “And with the sub-regions we’ve identified, we’ve provided a concrete neuroimaging tool to measure the sensitivity of signals associated with these phases that others can apply to their own data.”

For example, Treadway says, the new method could provide a window into how a drug is affecting the brains of patients suffering from motivational deficits, compared to controls.

The ventral striatum, located deep within the brain’s cerebral hemispheres, is an area associated with movement and mediating rewarding experiences and motivation.

Neuroimaging has consistently shown that the ventral striatum activates during decision-making to encode the potential value of rewards relative to costs, such as wait times and probability. The ventral striatum helps you decide whether to pay more for “next-day” delivery or choose “free, one-week” delivery to receive a package.

Neuroimaging studies had previously failed, however, to detect a strong value signal in the ventral striatum for decisions that require a physical effort. If you want more coffee, but the pot is empty, is it worth getting up and brewing some more?

“It was a mystery why this brain region encoded the value of a reward versus time and probability but did not appear to do so for physical effort,” Suzuki says. “It’s been a paradox in the neuroimaging literature.”

Imaging brains

Previous research on rodents showed that the ventral striatum is critical for motivating an animal to work for rewards like food. Animal research also shows evidence for two opposing signals in the ventral striatum. An activation signal prepares an animal to work and a discounting signal helps an animal select rewards that require the least effort. These signals help animals work for what they need, while also making sure they don’t work more than they have to.

The presence of these signals had never been tested in humans. The researchers theorized that as the physical cost to perform a task rises, the activation signal would drive an increase in activity in the ventral striatum, while the discounting signal would drive a decrease. They proposed that the simultaneous firing of these two signals—the cost of effort versus the activation of effort itself—is what made it harder to detect the value signal in previous studies.

An additional complication to detecting brain activation associated with physical effort is the fact that neuroimaging requires participants to lie still within a functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) machine while it scans their brains.

To get around these issues, the researchers designed fMRI experiments that would allow participants to remain in a supine position and would also separate the neural signals involving effort from the one associated with the cost of the effort.

For the first set of experiments, the researchers created a virtual maze. As their brains were scanned, study participants were presented with maze navigation tasks that required different levels of effort. In one condition, the participants watched themselves move through the virtual maze passively. In another condition, they simply pressed a button on a handheld device to move through the maze. A third condition required the higher effort of repeatedly and rapidly pressing the button to move through the maze. Each maze, when successfully completed, rewarded them with a nominal dollar amount.

During a second experiment, the neural activity of participants was measured as they made a series of choices between two options, with varying amounts of reward and effort required for each option. The effort and reward amounts were presented sequentially to try to isolate the effort-activation signal during the anticipation of various effort demands.

The results showed that two distinct regions of the ventral striatum fired in response to different phases of physical effort and effort-based decision making, with some overlap. Activity in an anterior region was mainly associated with reward and effort costs, while activity in a dorsal region was mainly associated with initiation of effortful movement. And this activity related to effortful movement was distinct from activity in another region, called the putamen, which was associated with initiation of simple movement.

Motivational deficits

The researchers now hope to build upon this increased awareness for how the brain encodes signals related to motivation.

“Our current paper provides a paradigm for how to measure brain activity for effort-based decisions associated with assigned tasks,” Suzuki says. “Now we’re developing experiments to identify specific modes of signaling when people spontaneously initiate action. That may give us a better measure of how the brain operates when people do things because they want to do them, in real-life situations.

“Getting sensitive measurements for how people normally decide to expend effort may help us develop better treatments for people suffering motivational deficits related to depression or other illnesses.”

Funding for the work was funded by grants from the National Institute for Mental Health and the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program. For the past three years, Treadway served as a paid consultant for Blackthorn Therapeutics and Avanir Pharmacetuicals, but neither of these entities supported the current work, which is solely the views of the authors.

Source: Emory University

The post How your brain decides to put in effort appeared first on Futurity.



from Futurity https://ift.tt/2Lr36i3

No comments:

Post a Comment